Press releases

The retina not only ensures sharp, but also stable vision

Researchers from Tübingen show for the first time that the milliseconds- long blindness that occurs during rapid movements of the eyes, which allows flicker-free vision, comes from the retina.

Anyone who has ever recorded a video knows that the camera must be held still so that the images do not blur. On the other hand, we always perceive a stable image of the world, even though our eyes constantly move back and forth when looking for new impressions and therefore cause shaky images to be projected onto the retina. However, because vision is suppressed for milliseconds during these brief eye movements, these blurring motions are not perceived. Professor Ziad M. Hafed and Dr. Thomas Münch and their teams show in the journal Nature Communications that the signal for this suppression comes directly from the retina and its neuronal activity. Hafed heads the "Active Perception" laboratory at the Hertie Institute for Clinical Brain Research (HIH), is a research group leader at the Werner Reichardt Center for Integrative Neuroscience (CIN) at the University of Tübingen, and a professor at the Faculty of Medicine. Münch is a group leader at the Research Institute for Ophthalmology at the University of Tübingen and at the CIN.

This is the first time that a coherent explanation has been found for the well-known phenomenon of saccadic suppression. Saccade is the term used to describe rapid movements of the eyes. So far, there have been two competing hypotheses about what causes saccadic suppression. According to the first hypothesis, the suppression starts from the motor signals that also trigger the eye movement. According to the second hypothesis, the suppression is purely visual and caused by the image sequences on the retina. This means that the blurred images would correct themselves already at the very first stage of vision, the retinal circuit. The results of the researchers show that the retinal hypothesis is indeed correct, but that the motor signal also plays an unexpected role.

Hafed, Münch, and their colleagues based their conclusions on various experiments. Initially, they asked subjects to look at high-resolution, computer-generated images of coarse and fine surfaces, which simulated the properties of natural images. They faded in short flashes of light of different strengths while the test subjects moved their eyes. Saccadic suppression made subjects unaware of the flashes, unless they were bright enough. It turned out that the ultra-short blindness was dependent on the displayed image. The suppression was stronger in the images with a coarse texture. It started earlier and lasted longer than in the images with a fine texture. "The fact that the strength and length of the suppression depended on the textures depicted could only mean that the trigger had to be visual," says Hafed. "We then confirmed this in many other experiments." Among other things, the scientists were able to show that the visual signal is already suppressed when it leaves the eye. “The retina therefore contributes directly to our stable visual impression,” says Münch. "It detects that the world is rushing by and regulates the sensitivity for a short time."

In order to explore the influence of the eye movement trigger itself, the scientists conducted an experiment in which the test subjects either moved their eyes over an object or kept their eyes still while the object was moving. The image sequences on the retina were the same in both cases, and saccadic suppression occurred in both cases. However, the suppression with real eye movements lasted much shorter. "We conclude that the movement signal makes a difference in the duration of saccadic suppression," Saad Idrees and Matthias Baumann explain. They are the two doctoral students who carried out the experiments. "So, the movement trigger is still relevant."

The results are also of medical relevance. Maybe they help people who can no longer see their world in a stable way due to illness. “This can be a very stressful symptom, for example in multiple sclerosis or Parkinson's disease,” says Hafed. "Perhaps special computer-controlled glasses can support these patients in the future to straighten the unstable pictures."

 

Original Publication

Idrees S., Baumann M.P., Franke F., Münch T.A., Hafed Z.M. (2020): Perceptual saccadic suppression starts in the retina, Nature Communications. 

https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-15890-w

 

Author contact

Prof. Dr. Ziad Hafed
Hertie Institute for Clinical Brain Research
Werner Reichardt Center for Integrative Neuroscience at the University of Tübingen
Otfried-Müller-Str. 25
72076 Tübingen

Tel: +49 7071 29-88819
Email: ziad.m.hafed@uni-tuebingen.de 

Dr. Thomas Münch
Research Institute for Ophthalmology at the University of Tübingen
Werner Reichardt Center for Integrative Neuroscience at the University of Tübingen

Otfried-Müller-Str. 25
72076 Tübingen

Email: thomas.muench@uni-tuebingen.de

 

Press Contact

Dr. Mareike Kardinal (on parental leave)
Head of Communications
Hertie Institute for Clinical Brain Research
Otfried-Müller-Str. 27
72076 Tübingen

Please contact

Dr. Hildegard Kaulen
Karlsruher Str. 8
65205 Wiesbaden

Tel: 06122 52718
E-Mail: h.k@kaulen-wissenschaft.de

 

 

Read the press release as a PDF

 

 

Press contact
Press contactProvisional Director of Communications
Silke DutzPhone 07071 29 88802silke.dutz@medizin.uni-tuebingen.deAddress

Hertie Institute for Clinical Brain Research
Director of Communications

Otfried-Müller-Str. 27
72076 Tübingen